Doi:10.1016/j.eururo.2006.03.057
Female Urology – Incontinence
Differential Effects of the Antimuscarinic Agents Darifenacin
and Oxybutynin ER on Memory in Older Subjects
Gary Kay Thomas Crook , Ludmyla Rekeda Raul Lima ,
Ursula Ebinger Miguel Arguinzoniz Michael Steel
Washington Neuropsychological Institute, Washington, DC, USA
Psychologix, Inc, Fort Lauderdale, FL, USA
Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation, East Hanover, NJ, USA
Novartis Pharma AG, Basel, Switzerland
Objectives: To investigate the effects of darifenacin controlled-release (CR)
Accepted March 13, 2006
and oxybutynin extended-release (ER) on cognitive function (particularly,
Published online ahead of
memory) in older subjects.
Methods: Healthy subjects (n = 150) 60 years were randomised to darifena-cin, oxybutynin ER or placebo in a multicentre, double-blind, double-
dummy, parallel-group, 3-week study. Doses were administered according
Cognitive function
to US labels: oxybutynin ER 10 mg once daily (od), increasing to 15 mg od then
20 mg od by week 3; darifenacin 7.5 mg od in weeks 1 and 2, then 15 mg od in
week 3. The primary end point was accuracy on the Name–Face Association
Test (delayed recall) at week 3.
Results: Results of the Name–Face Association Test at week 3 (mean differ-ence, 0.06, p = 0.908) showed no significant difference between darifenacinand placebo on delayed recall. In contrast, oxybutynin ER resulted in memoryimpairment, with significantly lower scores than placebo and darifenacin(mean differences, 1.30, p = 0.011 and 1.24, p = 0.022, respectively) fordelayed recall on the Name–Face Association Test at week 3. Additional testsof delayed recall indicated significant memory impairment with oxybutyninER versus placebo at certain time points, whereas darifenacin was similar toplacebo. No between-treatment differences were detected in self-rated mem-ory, demonstrating that subjects were unaware of memory deterioration.
Conclusions: While darifenacin had no significant effects on memory versusplacebo, oxybutynin ER caused significant memory deterioration (magnitudeof effect comparable to brain aging of 10 years). The results also demonstrate
UNCORRECTED PROOF
that subjects may not recognize/report memory deterioration.
# 2006 Published by Elsevier B.V.
* Corresponding author. Department of Neurology, Georgetown University School of Med-icine, Washington, DC 20008 USA. Tel. +1 202 686 7520; Fax: +1 202 686 8802.
E-mail address: (G. Kay).
0302-2838/$ – see back matter # 2006 Published by Elsevier B.V. doi:
opioids, benzodiazepines or sedating antihistamines), or
drugs that are substrates or inhibitors of cytochrome P450
Overactive bladder (OAB) is a widespread condition,
(CYP) 2D6 or CYP 3A4. Subjects were excluded if they suffered
the prevalence of which rises with increasing age
from conditions for which anticholinergic use is contra-
As bladder contractions are mediated primarily by
indicated (e.g., uncontrolled narrow angle glaucoma, urinaryretention), if they suffered from dementia or scored 27 on the
cholinergic activation of muscarinic M3 receptors
Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) or if they displayed evidence
antimuscarinics are used widely as first-line OAB
of depression (score 9 on Geriatric Depression Scale).
treatments Some agents may, however, be
Subjects entered a 2-week screening period, during which
associated with safety concerns, including effects
eligibility was assessed, and cognitive tests were administered
on the central nervous system (CNS); (e.g., memory
for familiarisation with procedures. Subjects were rando-
impairment) . The potential for CNS safety issues
mised (1:1:1 ratio) to receive once-daily (od) treatment with
is of particular concern in older patients, who are
oxybutynin ER, darifenacin or placebo (The 3-week
more vulnerable because of age-related memory
duration allowed titration of oxybutynin ER in accordance
decline reduced brain muscarinic receptor
with US prescribing information treatment for 1 week at
density and comorbidities . Furthermore,
each dose allowed steady-state cerebrospinal fluid concentra-
clinical studies have demonstrated increased sensi-
tions to be reached. Thus, the oxybutynin ER group received10 mg od in week 1, 15 mg od in week 2, and 20 mg od in week
tivity of older subjects to antimuscarinics, including
3. The darifenacin group received 7.5 mg od in weeks 1 and 2
effects on memory . Consequently, selecting an
(with a sham dose increase after 1 week), then 15 mg od during
appropriate antimuscarinic for OAB requires balan-
week 3 (in line with US prescribing information) . The third
cing efficacy with possible effects on memory.
group received placebo throughout, with sham dose increases
An important differentiator between antimuscari-
after 1 and 2 weeks. Blinding was maintained by using the
nics is activity at muscarinic receptor subtypes (M1–
double-dummy technique. Dosing was supervised during
M5). While oxybutynin binds preferentially to M3 and
week 3 to ensure compliance.
M1 receptors, darifenacin demonstrates 9.3-fold
Written informed consent was obtained, and the study was
selectivity for M
performed in accordance with good clinical practice guidelines
3 over M1 receptors in-vitro . This
observation may be important for effects on memory,
following ethical approval by a local review board according to
as the muscarinic M
the ethical principles laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki.
1 receptor plays a role in memory/
cognition . Indeed, it has been proposed that M3
Assessment of cognitive function
may confer benefits selectively, as non-M3-receptor–
mediated CNS side effects may be avoided (or
Cognitive function was assessed through the Psychologix/
reduced) Supporting evidence includes two
Cogscreen (Psychologix Inc, Fort Lauderdale, FL, USA; Cogsc-
studies of healthy subjects (one in subjects 65
reen, LLC, Washington, DC, USA) battery of computerised
years), in which darifenacin had no effect versus
cognitive function tests (CFTs) performed during clinic visits at
placebo on cognition . In contrast, a small-scale
baseline and following each week (prior to dose or sham dose
clinical study showed that oxybutynin was asso-
increase) shows the tests that were employed
ciated with cognitive dysfunction
(which have been demonstrated to be reliable and valid in
To our knowledge, there are no reports of a single-
numerous studies) tasks performed in each test and
controlled study investigating the effects of two
the sequence in which the tests were performed.
separate antimuscarinics on memory. We report a
comparison of the effects of darifenacin and
extended-release (ER) oxybutynin on memory in
older subjects.
Subjects and study design
Healthy male and female subjects aged
UNCORRECTED PROOF
60 years with English
as a first language (n = 150) were entered into a multicentre,
trolled, parallel-group, 3-week study. Subjects had to be able
to follow instructions and complete the computerised cogni-
tive tests with valid responses. Medications prohibited from 2
weeks prior to screening included drugs with anticholinergic
properties, drugs with known effects on cognition (e.g.,
Fig. 1 – Study design. ER = extended release.
Table 1 – Battery of tests used to assess memory and cognitive function
Immediate memory recall
Name–Face Association
Subjects are presented with a series of 14 people (displayed on
a video monitor) who introduce themselves individually bycommon first names. Subjects are then asked to recall nameswhen the 14 people reappear in a different sequence. Two separatetests performed (first and second acquisition).
First–Last Name Association
Subjects are presented with four pairs of first and last names.
Subjects are then asked to recall corresponding first namesas each last name is presented. Two separate testsperformed (first and second acquisition).
Facial Recognition
Subjects are presented with a single facial photograph and
are required to touch the face on the screen. In each of 24subsequent trials, subjects are required to identify a new faceadded to the set (8-second delay between each trial). Resultsare analysed as ‘correct before first miss' and ‘total correct'.
Delayed memory recall
Name–Face Association
30 minutes after completion of the immediate recall Name–Face
Association Test (as described above), subjects are re-presentedwith each face and asked for the corresponding name.
First–Last Name Association
30 minutes after completion of the immediate recall First–Last
Name Association Test (as described above), subjects are askedto recall corresponding first names as each last name isre-presented.
Misplaced Objects
Subjects are presented with a 12-room house on a monitor
and asked to place 20 objects within the house using a
touchscreen (no more than 2 objects per room).
After a 30-minute delay subjects are asked to recall objectplacement. Correct recall at first attempt is assessed.
Visual attention and memory
Matching to Sample
Subjects are presented with a checkerboard (4 4) made up of purple
and yellow squares. The checkerboard disappears and is replacedby one identical and one similar board. Subjects are asked toidentify the identical board. Response speed, accuracy and efficiencyare measured.
Visual Sequence Comparison
Subjects are presented with two random strings of numbers
and letters (4–8 items) simultaneously (shown on left andright hand sides of a monitor) and are asked to identify whetherthey are the same or different. For each pair of strings, differencesof up to two items are allowed. Speed, accuracy and efficiency(the number of problems correctly completed per minute) are measured.
Psychomotor/reaction time and
information processing
Divided Attention
Subjects watch a cursor (indicator) move vertically within a circle
(Visual Monitoring Alone)
divided into central, upper and lower sections. When the cursorcrosses into upper or lower sections, subjects are required topress a box marked ‘CENTRE' with a light pen. Indicator speedis measured as the median time the cursor spent outside thecentral section of the circle before the subject presses ‘CENTRE'.
Premature responses also are assessed.
Divided Attention
In the second component of the Divided Attention Test, the
(Visual Sequence Comparison and
Visual Sequence Comparison task (as described above) is
Visual Monitoring, Dual Condition)
UNCORRECTED PROOF
performed simultaneously with the Divided Attention IndicatorAlone Task (as described above). Response speed is measuredfor both tasks and accuracy and efficiency (number of itemscompleted) are measured for the Visual Sequence ComparisonTask in the Dual Condition (i.e., when performed with the DividedAttention Visual Monitoring Task). When the two tasks are presentedsimultaneously, the test assesses divided attention, working memory,and visual-motor and visual-perceptual speed. In addition,comparison of performance under single and dual task conditionsyields information regarding the subject's capacity for multitasking.
* Order in which tests were performed.
The primary end point was the effect of each antimus-
oxybutynin ER having an effect size, versus placebo, approxi-
carinic at week 3, versus placebo, on recent (delayed) memory
mately one third of that seen with scopolamine in older
as measured by accuracy on the delayed recall Name–Face
patients Allowing for a dropout rate of 30%, we continued
Association Test . This test measures an ability that
enrollment until at least 150 subjects (50% female) were
declines markedly with advancing age and has shown some
recruited. Thus, a 1:1:1 randomisation schedule gave approxi-
limited changes in response to drugs Name-recall is the
mately 50 subjects per group. Analysis of the primary end
most frequent memory complaint at all ages across multiple
point was based on a modified intent-to-treat (ITT) population
cultures This parameter is therefore relevant to the
(subjects taking at least one dose of study medication with
daily activities of older patients with OAB, making it
complete baseline and week 3 scores for the primary end
appropriate for analysis. The most important secondary end
point). For secondary end points, the modified ITT population
points were delayed recall on the First-Last Name Association
comprised subjects with scores for at least one test at baseline
Test and the Misplaced Objects Test both of which
and any post-baseline time point. Scores for active treatments
measure memory abilities relevant to daily life, and on which
were compared with placebo using an analysis of covariance
performance declines with advancing age. Also included as
(ANCOVA) model with baseline score, age and gender as
secondary measures were delayed recall scores at weeks 1 and
covariates. This was a two-sided test at the 5% significance
2, and effects on immediate memory, visual attention,
level. For exploratory purposes, comparisons between dar-
information processing and psychomotor/reaction time.
ifenacin and oxybutynin ER were derived from an identical
Subjective memory loss was assessed as a tertiary end
ANCOVA model.
point, using a validated self-reporting instrument, the
Memory Assessment Clinics Self Rating Scale (MAC-S) .
The MAC-S is a test in pencil/paper format, in which each
subject is asked to rate their abilities on 10 specific memory
tasks and two global items. Subjects were asked to rate how
their memory had changed since the beginning of the study.
One hundred fifty subjects (darifenacin n = 49, oxy-
Assessment of safety and tolerability
butynin ER n = 50, placebo n = 51) were randomised
and comprised the safety population. Of these, 134
Adverse events (AEs) (graded by severity and relationship to
completed the study and formed the modified ITT
study drug as assessed by investigators), including serious AEs
population for the primary analysis ). Of the
(SAEs), were documented. Results of laboratory tests and vital
nine subjects who discontinued in the darifenacin
signs were recorded.
group, six had partial data and were included in
secondary analyses. There were six discontinuations
Statistical analyses
in the oxybutynin ER group, of which partial data
A sample size of 35 subjects per group was considered
available for five subjects were included in secondary
sufficient to detect an effect size of 0.867 for active treatment
analyses. One subject in the placebo group discon-
versus placebo at week 3. This decision was based on
tinued, for whom partial data were not available for
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Fig. 2 – Patient flow through the 3-week study.
Table 2 – Subject demographics and baseline characteristics
Darifenacin (n = 49)
Oxybutynin ER (n = 50)
Mean age (yr) (range)
Mean BMI (kg/m2) (range)
Mean baseline score for delayed recall on
Name–Face Association
First–Last Name Association Testy
BMI = body mass index.
* Modified intent-to-treat population (primary): darifenacin n = 40, oxybutynin ER n = 44, placebo n = 50.
y Modified intent-to-treat population (secondary): darifenacin n = 46, oxybutynin ER n = 49, placebo n = 50.
inclusion in secondary analyses (). Demo-
(For oxybutynin ER, scores at week 2
graphics and baseline characteristics were similar
were significantly lower than for placebo or dar-
across treatment groups ().
ifenacin (mean differences, 0.99, p = 0.022 and
1.23, p = 0.007, respectively), showing that the
Assessment of memory—delayed recall
memory impairment at week 3 also was evident
at week 2 There were no significant
There was no significant difference between the
between–treatment differences at week 1, when
darifenacin and placebo groups with respect to the
lowest doses were administered.
primary end point, delayed recall on the Name–Face
For darifenacin and placebo, there was a trend for
Association Test at week 3 (mean difference, 0.06,
improvement during the study reflecting a
p = 0.908). In contrast, scores for delayed recall on
learning effect whereby subjects improve through
the Name–Face Association Test were significantly
practise. Thus, by week 3, mean scores for delayed
lower in the oxybutynin ER group than the placebo
recall on the Name–Face Association Test had
group (mean difference, 1.30, p = 0.011) or darife-
increased by 0.9 and 1.0 in the placebo and
nacin group (mean difference, 1.24, = 0.022), indi-
darifenacin groups, respectively. In the oxybutynin
cating memory deterioration (
ER group, in whom a similar learning effect was
Results from the Name–Face Association Test at
expected, a decrease in performance by 0.8 was
week 2 were consistent with those at week 3, when
there also was no significant difference between
In delayed recall on the First–Last Name Associa-
darifenacin and placebo groups for delayed recall
tion Test, oxybutynin ER resulted in significant
impairment versus placebo ( p < 0.05) at weeks 1 and
2 (). In contrast, no significant
differences were observed between darifenacin
and placebo at any time point ).
In the Misplaced Objects Test, oxybutynin ER
resulted in significantly lower scores than placebo at
weeks 2 and 3 for correct recall at first attempt
(suggesting a decline in performance,
whereas darifenacin was not significantly different
UNCORRECTED PROOF
from placebo at any time point.
Assessment of memory—immediate recall
Oxybutynin ER reduced accuracy scores for immedi-
ate recall on the First–Last Name Association Test at
Fig. 3 – Effects of darifenacin, oxybutynin ER and placebo onaccuracy of delayed recall on the Name–Face Association
second acquisition (attempt) versus placebo (mean
Test at each time point. ER = extended release;
differences, 0.28, 0.55 and 0.32 at weeks 1, 2 and
ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.
3, respectively), with significant effect at week
Table 3 – Accuracy of delayed recall on the Name–Face Association Test over
Estimated LSM difference
Darifenacin 7.5 mg od
Oxybutynin ER 10 mg od
Oxybutynin ER 10 mg od
Darifenacin 7.5 mg od
Oxybutynin ER 15 mg od
Oxybutynin ER 15 mg od
Darifenacin 15 mg od
Oxybutynin ER 20 mg od
Oxybutynin ER 20 mg od
ER = extended release; LSM = least square mean; od = once daily.
* Analysis of covariance model adjusted for baseline score, age and gender. Negative differences indicate relatively worse scores.
2 ( p = 0.029; No significant difference was
accuracy. Similarly in the Visual Sequence Compar-
detected between darifenacin and placebo on this
ison Test, there was no significant difference in
test ). No significant between–treatment
scores over time among treatment groups for
differences were noted for this test at first acquisi-
efficiency or accuracy.
tion (data not shown).
No significant difference was observed among
Information processing speed
treatment groups for other assessments of immedi-
ate recall: accuracy on Name–Face Association Test
(first or second acquisition) or accuracy on Facial
slower response times than placebo at week 3 for
Recognition Test (correct before first miss and total
sequence comparison speed in the Divided Atten-
tion Test (mean difference, 0.3 seconds, p = 0.012;
). There was no significant difference among
treatments in scores over time for sequence com-
parison efficiency or accuracy, and median reaction
No significant differences were observed between
to correct response in the Visual Sequence Compar-
treatment groups at any time point in the Matching
ison Test ().
to Sample Test for efficiency (speed or
At week 2, darifenacin had a significantly higher
score than oxybutynin ER for Single Task Premature
Hits (mean difference, 0.56, p = 0.046; but
was not significantly different from placebo. No
significant difference was observed among treat-
ment groups over time for Dual Task Reaction Time
or Dual Task Premature Hits.
UNCORRECTED PROOF
No significant difference was observed among
treatment groups in response speed to the Visual
Monitoring Task alone.
Memory assessment clinics self-rating scale
Fig. 4 – Effects of darifenacin, oxybutynin ER and placebo onaccuracy of delayed recall on the First–Last Name
In contrast with objective memory tests, there was
Association Test at each time point. ER = extended release;
no significant difference between groups in self-
ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.
rated memory, as assessed by MAC-S scores at any
Table 4 – in additional tests of memory and cognitive function over time
Estimated LSM difference
Immediate memory recall
Name-Face Association (accuracy, second acquisition)
First–Last Name Association (accuracy, second acquisition)
Facial Recognition (accuracy, correct before first miss)
Delayed memory recall
First–Last Name Association (accuracy)
Misplaced Objects (correct recall at first attempt)
Matching to Sample (efficiency)
Visual Sequence Comparison (efficiency)
Divided Attention (Sequence Comparison
Speed, Dual Condition) (s)
Divided Attention (Sequence Comparison Efficiency) (s)
Divided Attention (Sequence Comparison Accuracy) (s)
Visual Sequence Comparison (median reaction to
correct response)
Divided Attention (Single Task Premature Hits) (s)
Divided Attention (Task Reaction Time, Dual Condition) (s)
Divided Attention (Premature Hits, Dual Condition) (s)
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Divided Attention (Response Speed to
Visual Monitoring Task Alone) (s)
*Modified intent-to-treat population. ER = extended release; LSM = least square mean.
* p < 0.05.
y p < 0.01 (analysis of covariance [adjusted for baseline score, age and gender]).
Table 5 – Adverse event incidence (safety population)
Darifenacin (n = 49)
Oxybutynin ER (n = 50)
Subjects with any adverse event (n)
Severe adverse events
Serious adverse events
Most common all-causality adverse events (n)
All-causality nervous system events (n)
time point. At week 3, the mean MAC-S scores were
tests of delayed recall indicated significant memory
40.2, 41.8 and 40.2 for darifenacin, oxybutynin ER
impairment with oxybutynin ER versus placebo
and placebo, respectively, which were similar to
(Name–Face Association at week 2, First–Last Name
baseline (40.7, 40.0 and 39.1, respectively).
Association at weeks 1 and 2, and Misplaced Objects
at weeks 2 and 3), while darifenacin was not
significantly different from placebo in delayed recall
at any time point.
The incidence of all-causality AEs is shown in
The delayed recall tests were selected on the basis
The most frequently reported AEs, as
of their relevance to daily activities. Recalling the
expected for this class, were dry mouth and
name of someone to whom one is introduced is the
constipation. Dry mouth occurred more frequently
most problematic memory task faced on a daily
during oxybutynin ER than darifenacin treatment
basis in many cultures, and performance declines
(40.0% vs 26.5%). One patient in each of the
markedly over the adult life-span . For exam-
oxybutynin ER and darifenacin groups discontinued
ple, performance on the Name–Face Association
because of dry mouth. The incidence of constipation
Test declines by >65% between age 25 and 75
was higher in the darifenacin than oxybutynin ER
years This ‘normal' decline may be exaggerated
group ). Only one patient (in the darifenacin
by drugs and the combined effect would be
group) discontinued because of constipation. The
expected to be of clear clinical significance. In a
total incidence of all-causality nervous system
similar manner, performance declines with age on
events was similarly low in all groups, with only
the First–Last Name Association and Misplaced
two severe cases, both in the oxybutynin ER group
Objects Tests ; this effect also can be exaggerated
). There was one serious AE (hip fracture
by drugs . Thus, the deleterious effects of
following an accident at home in a subject given
oxybutynin ER on these tests suggests that this
oxybutynin ER), which was not considered to be
agent, at the dosage tested, may be associated with
related to the study drug. There were no clinically
diminished performance on important tasks of daily
significant findings from assessments of laboratory
life that depend on delayed recall.
values or vital signs.
Differential outcomes between darifenacin and
oxybutynin ER may arise from differences in either
CNS penetration or muscarinic receptor–binding
profiles. Darifenacin exhibits limited CNS penetra-
tion in preclinical studies, which may result from its
This study demonstrated that the M3 selective
moderate lipophilicity, relatively large molecular
receptor antagonist darifenacin had no significant
size, polarity and active efflux across the blood-
effect on memory in older
UNCORRECTED PROOF
subjects. In contrast,
brain barrier via the P-glycoprotein pump Once
oxybutynin ER resulted in significant memory
in the CNS, muscarinic-binding profiles play a role.
deterioration, as measured by delayed recall on
Whereas darifenacin shows marked M3 selectivity,
the Name–Face Association Test at week 3. Compar-
oxybutynin demonstrates high affinity for M3 and
ing these results with normative data for this test
M1 subtypes The latter subtype is abundant in
indicates that the degree of memory change seen
the neocortex, hippocampus and neostriatum, in
with oxybutynin ER (baseline to week 3) was
contrast with low levels of M3 receptors in the brain
comparable to a decline that occurs over the course
and M1 receptors are known to be particularly
of 10 years in the normal aging process. Additional
hypothesis is supported by the low incidence of
mon in older patients in clinical practice . In the
nervous system AEs with darifenacin, both here and
study reported here, in patients who were not
in longer-term clinical trials. A pooled analysis of
receiving anticholinergic co-medication and had
three 12-week, fixed-dose studies with darifenacin
no cognitive impairment at baseline, the lowest
showed a profile of nervous system events that was
oxybutynin ER dose (10 mg od) did not cause
comparable with placebo, both overall and in
memory impairment. In clinical practice, however,
patients 65 years In contrast, results from
when patients may be receiving concomitant antic-
four clinical studies of 4 months showed that the
holinergics or have existing memory impairment, it
incidence of somnolence and dizziness with oxybu-
is possible that this dose of oxybutynin ER may have
tynin ER 5–30 mg/day was 12% and 6%, respectively
a greater impact.
This study also measured immediate recall, for
which oxybutynin ER showed some impairment
versus placebo, while darifenacin and placebo were
not significantly different. Major changes were not
The results of this study add considerably to our
expected here, as muscarinic receptor activation is
knowledge regarding the differential effects of two
thought to be involved primarily in memory con-
antimuscarinics, darifenacin and oxybutynin ER, on
solidation (i.e., how recent recollections are
memory. The finding that darifenacin does not
crystallised into memory). Similarly, significant
impair memory is consistent with earlier studies
effects were not expected in attention tests, and
and the observation assumes clinical sig-
no effects were observed across multiple tests.
nificance in light of the clear demonstration of
Within the Divided Attention Test, however, dar-
memory impairment during oxybutynin ER treat-
ifenacin was significantly worse than placebo for
ment. These findings highlight a need for further
sequence comparison speed (mean difference,
studies to fully establish the effects of all OAB
0.3 seconds) but did not differ from placebo in
antimuscarinics on memory/cognition.
accuracy. Darifenacin scored worse than oxybuty-
nin ER in the number of premature hits on a reaction
Conflicts of interest
time measure at week 2 (but not week 3) and did not
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by
differ significantly from placebo. Given that subjects
an educational grant from Novartis Pharma AG, and
receiving darifenacin showed no detriment on
editorial and project management services were
multiple other tests of attention, we did not consider
provided by ACUMED1.
these isolated findings clinically relevant. In con-
trast, findings with oxybutynin ER on memory were
replicated across multiple tests at different time
points, and are supported by earlier studies and an
identifiable mechanism of action.
We are grateful for the support of our co-investiga-
Interestingly, there were no reported differences
tors, J. Diaz, FL, USA; L. Gilderman, FL, USA; J. Miller,
in self-rated memory between treatments. This
FL, USA; J. Nardandrea, FL, USA; B. Rankin, FL, USA;
finding is particularly important as it indicates that
M. Sabbagh, AZ, USA; L. Schmidt, AZ, USA, and to the
memory changes may go unnoticed. This low level
staff of Network Neurometrics, Inc, and Advanced
of awareness may account for the low rate of
Research Corporation who conducted the study.
reporting of memory impairment with antimuscari-
nic therapies in clinical practice. In addition,
disease- or treatment-related memory/cognitive
impairment may be difficult to recognise, and
memory decline may be attributed wrongly to aging.
[1] Milsom I, Abrams P, Cardozo L, Roberts RG, Thuroff J,
Although relevant for all
UNCORRECTED PROOF
patients, these findings
Wein AJ. How widespread are the symptoms of an over-
are particularly important for older patients, since
active bladder and how are they managed? A population-
this population may have risk factors for memory/
based prevalence study. BJU Int 2001;87:760–6.
cognitive impairment Important differences
[2] Chess-Williams R, Chapple CR, Yamanishi T, Yasuda K,
between this study and clinical practice are that frail
Sellers DJ. The minor population of M3 receptors mediate
elderly patients may have been under-represented
contraction of human detrusor muscle in vitro. J Auton
(since ability to complete computerised cognitive
tests and normal MMSE scores were required), and
[3] Ouslander JG. Management of overactive bladder. N Engl J
polymedication with antimuscarinics will be com-
[4] Crook TH, Lebowitz D, Pirozzolo FJ, et al. Recalling names
after introduction: changes across the adult life span in
two cultures. Development Neuropsychol 1993;9:103–13.
[18] Crook 3rd TH, Larrabee GJ. A self-rating scale for evaluat-
[5] Norbury R, Travis MJ, Erlandsson K, et al. SPET imaging of
ing memory in everyday life. Psychol Aging 1990;5:48–57.
central muscarinic receptors with (R,R) [123I]–I–QNB:
[19] Youngjohn JR, Larrabee GJ, Crook TH. First-last Names
methodological considerations. Nucl Med Biol 2004;31:
and Grocery List Selective Reminding Test: two compu-
terized measures of everyday verbal learning. Arch Clin
[6] Sunderland T, Tariot PN, Cohen RM, Weingartner H,
Mueller 3rd EA, Murphy DL. Anticholinergic sensitivity
[20] Crook TH, West RL. Name recall performance across the
in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type and
adult life-span. Br J Psychol 1990;81:335–49.
agematched controls. A dose response study. Arch Gen
[21] Nickelsen T, Lufkin EG, Riggs BL, Cox DA, Crook TH.
Raloxifene hydrochloride, a selective estrogen receptor
[7] Flicker C, Ferris SH, Serby M. Hypersensitivity to scopola-
modulator: safety assessment of effects on cognitive
mine in the elderly. Psychopharmacology 1992;107:437–41.
function and mood in postmenopausal women. Psycho-
[8] Molchan SE, Martinez RA, Hill JL, et al. Increased cognitive
sensitivity to scopolamine with age and a perspective on
[22] Crook 3rd TH, Youngjohn JR, Larrabee GJ, Salama M. Aging
the scopolamine model. Brain Res Brain Res Rev
and everyday memory: a cross-cultural study. Neuropsy-
[9] Napier C, Gupta P. Darifenacin is selective for the human
[23] Crook 3rd TH, Youngjohn JR, Larrabee GJ. The misplaced
recombinant M3 receptor subtype. Neurourol Urodyn
objects test: a measure of everyday visual memory. J Clin
Exp Neuropsychol 1990;12:819–33.
[10] Anagnostaras SG, Murphy GG, Hamilton SE, et al. Selec-
[24] Coffey DJ, Jenkyn LR, Coffey AK, Wells BB. Sertraline vs
tive cognitive dysfunction in acetylcholine M1 muscarinic
amitriptyline vs placebo: effects on cognitive and motor
receptor mutant mice. Nat Neurosci 2003;6:51–8.
functioning in the elderly. Neuropsychopharmacology
[11] Drachman DA, Noffsinger D, Sahakian BJ, Kurdziel S, Flem-
ing P. Aging, memory, and the cholinergic system: A study
[25] Skerjanec A, Devineni D. The clinical pharmacokinetics of
of dichotic listening. Neurobiol Aging 1980;1:39–43.
darifenacin. Clin Pharmacokinet. In press.
[12] Andersson K-E. Potential benefits of muscarinic M3 recep-
[26] Volpicelli LA, Levey AI. Muscarinic acetylcholine receptor
tor selectivity. Eur Urol Suppl 2002;1:23–8.
subtypes in cerebral cortex and hippocampus. Prog Brain
[13] Kay GG, Wesnes KA. Pharmacodynamic effects of darife-
nacin, a muscarinic M3 selective receptor antagonist for
[27] Chapple C, Steers W, Norton P, Millard R, Kralidis G,
the treatment of overactive bladder, in healthy volun-
Glavind K, et al. A pooled analysis of three phase III
teers. BJU Int 2005;96:1055–62.
studies to investigate the efficacy, tolerability and safety
[14] Lipton RB, Kolodner K, Wesnes K. Assessment of cognitive
of darifenacin, a muscarinic M3 selective receptor antago-
function of the elderly population: effects of darifenacin. J
nist, in the treatment of overactive bladder. BJU Int
[15] Katz IR, Sands LP, Bilker W, DiFilippo S, Boyce A, D'Angelo
[28] Foote J, Glavind K, Kralidis G, Wyndaele JJ. Treatment of
K. Identification of medications that cause cognitive
overactive bladder in the older patient: pooled analysis of
impairment in older people: the case of oxybutynin chlor-
three Phase III studies of darifenacin, an M3 selective
ide. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998;46:8–13.
receptor antagonist. Eur Urol 2005;48:471–7.
[16] Ditropan XL1 (oxybutynin chloride) Extended Release
[29] Power AE, Vazdarjanova A, McGaugh JL. Muscarinic cho-
Tablets. Prescribing Information. Ortho-McNeil Pharma-
linergic influences in memory consolidation. Neurobiol
ceutical, Inc., June 2004 (available at:
Learn Mem 2003;80:178–93.
[30] Scheife R, Takeda M. Central Nervous System Safety of
[17] Enablex1 (darifenacin) Extended Release Tablets. Pre-
anticholinergic drugs for the treatment of overactive
scribing Information. Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corpora-
bladder in the elderly. Clin Ther 2005;27:144–53.
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Source: http://staging.cogres.com/Content/PDFs/2006DifferentialEffectsOfTheAntimuscarinicAgentsDarifenacinAndOxybutyninEROnMemoryInOlderSubjects.pdf
themedicalroundtable.biz
This electronic representation of FoxP2 Media LLC/The Medical Roundtable intellectual property is provided for noncommercial use only. Unauthorized posting of FoxP2 Media LLC/The Medical Roundtable electronic documents to a non-FoxP2 Media LLC website is prohibited. Prior written permission is required from FoxP2 Media LLC to reproduce, or reuse in another form, any of our documents for commercial use.
Microsoft word - 3295.doc
LIGHT DIAGNOSTICS™ Cat. No. 3295 FOR IN VITRO DIAGNOSTIC USE Authorised. Representative: Millipore (UK) Ltd. Fleming Road, Kirkton Campus Livingston EH54 7BN UK. Tel +44 1506 404000 Fax +44 1506 404073 28820 Single Oak Drive Temecula, CA 92590 • USA & Canada Phone: +1(800) 437-7500 • Fax: +1 (951) 676-9209 • ISO Registered Worldwide www.chemicon.com • [email protected] • [email protected]